GE 141: SPATIAL ORGANIZATION.
INTRODUCTION.
The lead section of this article may need to be rewritten. (February 2013) |
Contents
Spatial organization in eusocial insects
Individuals in a social insect colony can be spatially organized, or arranged non-randomly inside the nest. These miniature territories, or spatial fidelity zones have been described in honey bees (Apis mellifera[4]), ants (Odontomachus brunneus;[5] Temnothorax albipennis;[6] Pheidole dentata[3]), and paper wasps (Polistes dominulus,[7] Ropalidia revolutionalis[8]). While residing in these zones, workers perform the task appropriate to the area they reside. For example, individuals that remain in the center of an ant nest are more likely to feed larvae, whereas individuals found at the periphery of the nest are more likely to forage.[5][6] E. O. Wilson proposed that by remaining in small, non-random areas inside the nest, the distance an individual moves between tasks may be minimized, and overall colony efficiency would increase.[3]Spatial organization in the nest
"Foraging-for-work"
There are a variety of ways in which individuals can divide space inside a nest. According to the “foraging-for-work” hypothesis, adult workers begin performing tasks in the area of the nest where they emerged, and gradually move towards the periphery of the nest as demands to perform particular tasks change. This hypothesis is based on two observations: "(1) that there is spatial structure in the layout of tasks in social insect colonies and (2) that workers first become adults in or around the center of the nest".[9] Individuals can remain in an area for an extended period of time, as long as tasks need to be performed there. Over time, an individual’s zone may shift as tasks are accomplished and workers search for other areas where tasks need to be performed. Honey bees, for example, begin their adult life caring for brood located in the area near where they emerged (i.e. nurse bees). Eventually, workers move away from the brood rearing area and begin to perform other tasks, such as food storage, guarding, or foraging.[4]Dominance hierarchy
Space inside the nest may also be divided as a result of dominance interactions. For example, in paper wasp colonies, a single inseminated queen may found (initiate) a colony after waking up from hibernation (overwintering). However, it is common in many species that multiple inseminated females join these foundresses instead of founding their own nest.[10] When multiple inseminated females found a colony together, the colony grows quickly, yet only one individual will become the primary egg-layer.[11] Through a series of dominance interactions, the most aggressive wasp will emerge as the dominant individual and will become the primary egg-layer for the group (the prime role for ensuring your genes are passed on to subsequent generations), whereas the remaining subordinate wasps will perform other tasks, such as nest construction or foraging.[8] There is evidence that these dominance interactions affect the spatial zones individuals occupy as well. In paper wasps (Ropalidia revolutionalis), as well as in the ant species Odontomachus brunneus,[5] dominant individuals are more likely to reside in the central areas of the nest, where they take care of brood, while the subordinate individuals are pushed towards the edge, where they are more likely to forage. It is unknown whether division of space or establishment of dominance occurs first and if the other is a result of it.Spatial organization outside the nest
There is also evidence that foragers, which are the insects that leave the nest to collect the valuable resources for the developing colony, can divide space outside the nest. Makino & Sakai showed that bumble bee foragers maintain foraging zones in flower patches, which means that bees consistently return to the same areas within a patch and there is little overlap between individuals.[12] These zones can expand and contract when neighboring foragers are removed or introduced, respectively.[13] By dividing foraging patches into miniature ‘foraging territories’, individuals can maximize the number of flowers visited with minimal interruptions or competition between foragers. These ‘foraging territories’ divided among individuals from the same colony are the result of self-organization among the foragers; that is, there is no lead forager dictating where the bees will forage. Instead, the maintenance of these foraging zones is due to simple rules followed by each individual forager. Studies to determine these “rules” are an important area of research in computer science, basic biology, behavioral ecology, and mathematic modeling.Spatial organization as an emergent property of a self-organized system
The self-organization observed in foraging territories is a microcosm for the self-organization seen in the entire colony. Spatial organization observed across social insect colonies can be considered an emergent property of a self-organized complex system. It is self-organized because there is no leader dictating where each individual will reside, nor which task an individual will perform once they get there.[14] Instead, zones may be a by-product of division of labor, whereby individuals end up in a particular location for a period of time based on the task they perform,[9] or dominance interactions, whereby dominant individuals are granted access to the most desirable places inside the nest.[5][8] Spatial patterns exhibited by individuals of social insect colonies are not obvious, because it is difficult to observe and differentiate among individuals inside a nest cavity or flying across a foraging patch. However, when careful attention is given to the individual worker, the spatial organization of workers in the nest becomes apparent.See also
References
- Ball, P. The Self-Made Tapestry: Pattern formation in nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-850244-3.
- Hamilton, W.D. (1971). "Geometry for the selfish herd". Journal of Theoretical Biology 31 (2): 295–311. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(71)90189-5. PMID 5104951.
- Wilson, E. O. (1976). "Behavioral discretization and the number of castes in an ant species". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 1 (2): 141–154. doi:10.1007/BF00299195.
- Seeley, T. D. (1982). "Adaptive significance of the age polyethism schedule in honeybee colonies". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 11 (4): 287–293. doi:10.1007/BF00299306.
- Powell, S.; Tschinkel, W. R. (1999). "Ritualized conflict in Odontomachus brunneus and the generation of interaction-based task allocation: a new organizational mechanism in ants". Animal Behaviour 58 (5): 965–972. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1238. PMID 10564598.
- Sendova-Franks, A. B.; Franks, N. R. (1995). "Spatial relationships within nests of the ant Leptothorax unifasciatus (Latr.) and their implications for the division of labour". Animal Behaviour 50: 121–136. doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0226.
- Baracchi, D; Zaccaroni, M, Cervo R, Turillazzi S (2010). "Home Range Analysis in the Study of Spatial Organization on the Comb in the Paper Wasp Polistes Dominulus". Ethology 116 (7): 579–587. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01770.x.
- Robson, SKA; Bean, K, Hansen, J, Norling, K, Rowe, RJ, White, D (2000). "Social and spatial organization in colonies of a primitively eusocial wasp Ropalidia revolutionalis (de Saussure) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)". Aust J Entomol 39: 20–24. doi:10.1046/j.1440-6055.2000.00135.x.
- Franks, NR; Tofts, C. (1994). "Foraging for work: how tasks allocate workers". Animal Behaviour 48 (2): 470–472. doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1261.
- Wilson, E. O. (1971). The Insect Societies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- West-Eberhard, M. J. (1969). "The social biology of Polistine wasps". Miscellaneous Publications Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan 140: 1–101.
- Makino, TT; Sakai, S (2004). "Findings on spatial foraging patterns of bumblebees (Bombus ignitus) from a bee-tracking experiment in a net cage". Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56 (2): 155–163. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0773-x.
- Makino, TT,; Sakai, S. (2005). "Does interaction between bumblebees (Bombus ignitus) reduce their foraging area?: Bee-removal experiments in a net cage". Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57 (6): 617–622. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0877-3.
- Camazine, S.; Deneubourg, J.-L., Franks, N. R., Sneyd, J., Theraulaz, G., & Bonabeau, E. (2001). Self-Organization in Biological Systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
No comments:
Post a Comment